EXPLORING CONNECTION BETWEEN EMPATHY, NEUROHORMONES AND AGGRESSION
Empathy is typically
seen as eliciting warmth and compassion -- a generally positive state that makes
people do good things to others. However, empathy may also motivate aggression
on behalf of the vulnerable other. Researchers at the State University of New
York at Buffalo, examined whether assessed or elicited empathy would lead to
situation-specific aggression on behalf of another person, and to explore the
potential role of two neurohormones in explaining a connection between empathy
and aggression. The study is published in Personality
and Social Psychology Bulletin.
Design of the Study
Empathic impulses
are aimed at reducing the suffering of the target of empathy. Sometimes
aggression may be the response that is perceived to best address the need of
the other, or best suited to end their suffering. This effect may, in part, be
due in part to physiological changes that occur in the body as a result of
empathy. The research focused on two neurohormones, oxytocin and vasopressin.
Oxytocin has been associated with empathy in previous research, and also with
protective aggression. Vasopressin has been much more commonly studied in the
animal literature, but has similarly been associated with aggression to defend
a mate or offspring.
The first study
asked participants to write and answer questions about a time in the past 12
months where they witnessed a close other being hurt physically or emotionally
by a third party other than themselves. The results illustrate that empathy,
not trait aggression or perceptions of emotional threat toward the self,
motivated predicted aggression of the participants.
The second study
involved an empathy manipulation and a distress manipulation. Participants were
given a scenario describing someone having financial difficulty, and that
person was either worried (high need) or not (low need). Half the participants
were told to read the scenario with instructions that were empathy-inducing,
and half were not. Participants were also told this person would engage in a
competitive task with another individual, and participants were given the
opportunity to sabotage the performance of the other individual by assigning
that person a certain amount of hot sauce to drink. "Hot sauce was
described to them as a clearly painful and performance hindering substance,
meaning that the more hot sauce they assigned, the worse the anonymous person
would do on the task…and presumably, the more likely that the person with
financial troubles could win," explains lead researcher Anneke Buffone.
Results of the Study
Participants who
felt empathy in the Study 1 were more likely to aggress against the close
other's perpetrator if the close other was perceived to be distressed, but not
when the close other was not perceived to be distressed. The empathy
manipulation in the Study 2 increased aggression (the amount of hot sauce
assigned) toward the target's competitor, but only when the empathy target was
described as distressed. The results of study 2 demonstrate that empathy-linked
aggression can occur for a stranger, and that provocation by the target is
unlikely to be the sole mechanism for empathy-linked aggression.
The participants
contributed saliva samples for analysis of their neurohormone gene variants. In
both studies, participants with a short/short version of the 1a vasopressin
receptor (AVPR1a) showed less aggression, while those with a long version of
the receptor showed higher aggression. The pattern is consistent with the
possibility that vasopressin facilitates empathic responses, including
aggression, to individuals in need. In one study, individuals with one oxytocin
receptor genotype, OXTR rs53576 GG showed greater aggression than those with
the AA/AG genotype.
The study ruled out
certain variables, such as trait aggression and impulsiveness. "Aggression
is known to result from characteristics such as impulsiveness, trait
aggression, trait or state anger. We wanted to rule out these motivators of
aggression because our argument is that anyone can act aggressively out of an
empathic impulse, not just those with a certain personality," Buffone
elaborates. "We think that among situational motivators of aggression,
witnessing the suffering or need of others people have come to care about has
been largely overlooked."
The findings of the
research provide evidence that activating empathy may prompt aggression toward
those in conflict or competition with empathy targets, even independent of
traditional predictors of aggression and in the absence of wrongdoing or
provocation from the target of aggression. Empathy could even lead an
individual to blame an innocent person for a crime or misdeed to protect a
friend or child from punishment. And it is even possible that feeling empathy
for strangers perceived to be treated unjustly might motivate aggression on
their behalf. In all of these cases, empathy can lead more directly to
aggression -- anger isn't always necessary.
Comments
Post a Comment