GENETIC TWEAK GAVE YELLOW FEVER MOSQUITOES A NOSE FOR HUMAN ODOR
One of the world's
deadliest mosquitoes sustains its taste for human blood thanks in part to a
genetic tweak that makes it more sensitive to human odor, according to new
research.
Researchers report in
the journal Nature that the yellow fever mosquito contains a
version of an odor-detecting gene in its antennae that is highly attuned to
sulcatone, a compound prevalent in human odor. The researchers found that the
gene, AaegOr4, is more abundant and more sensitive in the human-preferring
"domestic" form of the yellow fever mosquito than in its ancestral
"forest" form that prefers the blood of non-human animals.
The research provides
a rare glimpse at the genetic changes that cause behaviors to evolve, explained
first author Carolyn "Lindy" McBride, an assistant professor in
Princeton University's Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology and the Princeton
Neuroscience Institute who conducted the work as a postdoctoral researcher at
the Rockefeller University. Uncovering the genetic basis of changes in behavior
can help us understand the neural pathways that carry out that behavior,
McBride said.
The research also
could help in developing better ways to stem the yellow fever mosquito's
appetite for humans, McBride said. The yellow fever mosquito is found in
tropical and subtropical areas worldwide and is the principal carrier of yellow
fever, the measles-like dengue fever, and the painful infection known as
chikungunya. Yellow fever annually kills tens of thousands of people worldwide,
primarily in Africa, while dengue fever infects hundreds of millions. The
research also suggests a possible genetic root for human preference in other
mosquitoes, such as malaria mosquitoes, although that species is genetically
very different from the yellow fever mosquito.
"The more we know
about the genes and compounds that help mosquitoes target us, the better chance
we have of manipulating their response to our odor" McBride said, adding
that scent is not the only driver of mosquito behavior, but it is a predominant
factor.
The researchers first
conducted a three-part series of experiments to establish the domestic yellow
fever mosquito's preference for human scent. Forest and domestic mosquitoes
were put into a large cage and allowed to bite either a guinea pig or a
researcher's arm. Then the mosquitoes were allowed to choose between streams of
air that had passed over a guinea pig or human arm. Finally, to rule out
general mosquito attractants such as exhaled carbon dioxide, mosquitoes were
allowed to choose between the scent of nylon sleeves that had been in contact
with a human or a guinea pig.
In all three cases,
the domestic form of the yellow fever mosquito showed a strong preference for
human scent, while the forest form primarily chose the guinea pig. Although
domestic mosquitoes would sometimes go for the guinea pig, it happened very
rarely, McBride said.
McBride and colleagues
then decided to look for differences in the mosquitoes' antennae, which are
equivalent to a human's nose. They interbred domestic and forest mosquitoes,
then interbred their offspring to create a second hybrid generation. The
genomes of these second-generation hybrids were so completely reshuffled that
when the researchers compared the antennae of the human- and guinea
pig-preferring individuals they expected to see only genetic differences linked
directly to behavior, McBride said.
The researchers found
14 genes that differed between human- and guinea pig-preferring hybrids -- two
of them were the odorant receptors Or4 and Or103. Choosing to follow up on Or4,
the researchers implanted the gene into fruit-fly neurons. They found that the
neurons exhibited a burst of activity when exposed to sulcatone, but no change
when exposed to guinea pig odors. McBride plans to further study Or103 and
other genes that could be linked to host preference at Princeton.
This work provides
insight into how the domestic form of the yellow fever mosquito evolved from
its animal-loving ancestor into a human-biting specialist, McBride said.
"At least one of the things that happened is a retuning of the ways odors
are detected by the antennae," she said. "We don't yet know whether
there are also differences in how odor information is interpreted by the
brain."
Comments
Post a Comment