ELDERLY BRAINS LEARN , BUT MAY BE TOO MUCH
A new study led by
Brown University reports that older learners retained the mental flexibility
needed to learn a visual perception task but were not as good as younger people
at filtering out irrelevant information.
The findings undermine
the conventional wisdom that the brains of older people lack flexibility, or
"plasticity," but highlight a different reason why learning may
become more difficult as people age: They learn more than they need to.
Researchers call this the "plasticity and stability dilemma." The new
study suggests older people may indeed be facing it.
"Plasticity may
be kept OK, in contrast with the view of many researchers on aging who have
said that the degree of plasticity of older people gets lower," said Takeo
Watanabe, the Fred M. Seed Professor at Brown University, corresponding author
of the study inCurrent Biology. "However, we have found that the
stability is problematic. Our learning and memory capability is limited. You
don't want older, existing important information that is already stored to be
replaced with trivial information."
Numerals, not dots
To conduct the study,
Watanabe and his team enrolled a group of 10 people between 67 and 79 years old
and another group of 10 people ages 19 to 30 for an experiment. Over a nine-day
period, they trained on a simple visual exercise: Shown a quick sequence of six
symbols -- four letters and two numerals -- volunteers were asked to report the
numerals they saw. Their performance on a test at the end of training was
compared to their score on a pre-test.
The volunteers were
explicitly instructed to only bother with spotting the two numerals, but each
symbol they saw had a background of moving dots. Unbeknownst to the subjects,
those dots would move with varying degrees of cohesiveness of direction. In the
pre- and post-tests the researchers also asked the volunteers to report the
direction of dot movement when they saw the numerals.
The results of the
testing were telling. Older people improved as much as younger people on the
relevant task of identifying the two numerals.
"These results
indicate that older subjects as well as younger subjects showed significant
amounts of task-relevant learning," the authors wrote. "No evidence
was obtained that indicates that older individuals have a problem with
plasticity."
Last week, in fact,
Watanabe and colleagues published a study showing that plasticity during visual
learning occurs in older people as well as younger ones, but it is manifest
differently in the brains of the different groups.
But in this study when
it came to the irrelevant skill of discerning the prevailing direction of dot
movement, older people learned that, too, even when it was at its most obvious.
Younger people, meanwhile, only showed improvement on discerning movement when
it was insidiously subtle. If it was clear, they recognized it and filtered it
out.
The idea that the most
obvious signals were the most easily filtered, suggested that the difference
between older and younger learners was a matter of attention.
The researchers
therefore subjected the volunteers to another test for the ability to find a
relevant stimulus amid a number of distractors. Older people did notably worse
than younger ones, adding evidence that the attentional systems for filtering
out irrelevant stimuli were indeed weaker in older learners. Importantly, the
poorer an older subject was at the ability to filter out irrelevant stimuli,
the more irrelevant stimuli the subject learned.
Watanabe said the
finding is not necessarily discouraging news. Perhaps filtering can be improved
with some kind of training.
"The hope is that
maybe what older people need to do is to learn a skill to avoid learning what
is not necessary," he said.
Comments
Post a Comment