ELEMENTS OF SURPRISE HELPS BABIES LEARN
Infants have innate
knowledge about the world and when their expectations are defied, they learn
best, researchers at Johns Hopkins University found.
In a paper to be
published April 3 in the journalScience, cognitive psychologists Aimee
E. Stahl and Lisa Feigenson demonstrate for the first time that babies learn
new things by leveraging the core information they are born with. When
something surprises a baby, like an object not behaving the way a baby expects
it to, the baby not only focuses on that object, but ultimately learns more
about it than from a similar yet predictable object.
"For young
learners, the world is an incredibly complex place filled with dynamic stimuli.
How do learners know what to focus on and learn more about, and what to ignore?
Our research suggests that infants use what they already know about the world to
form predictions. When these predictions are shown to be wrong, infants use
this as a special opportunity for learning," said Feigenson, a professor
of psychological and brain sciences in the university's Krieger School of Arts
and Sciences. "When babies are surprised, they learn much better, as
though they are taking the occasion to try to figure something out about their
world."
The two researchers'
study involved four experiments with preverbal 11-month-old babies, designed to
determine whether babies learned more effectively about objects that defied
their expectations. If they did, researchers wondered if babies would also seek
out more information about surprising objects and if this exploration meant
babies were trying to find explanations for the objects' strange behavior.
First the researchers
showed the babies both surprising and predictable situations regarding an
object. For instance, one group of infants saw a ball roll down a ramp and
appear to be stopped by a wall in its path. Another group saw the ball roll
down the ramp and appear to pass -- as if by magic -- right through the wall.
When the researchers
gave the babies new information about the surprising ball, the babies learned
significantly better. In fact, the infants showed no evidence of learning about
the predictable ball. Furthermore, the researchers found that the babies chose
to explore the ball that had defied their expectations, even more than toys
that were brand new but had not done anything surprising.
The researchers found
that the babies didn't just learn more about surprising objects -- they wanted
to understand them. For instance, when the babies saw the surprising event in
which the ball appeared to pass through the wall, they tested the ball's
solidity by banging it on the table. But when babies saw a different surprising
event, in which the ball appeared to hover in midair, they tested the ball's
gravity by dropping it onto the floor. These results suggest that babies were
testing specific hypotheses about the objects' surprising behavior.
"The infants'
behaviors are not merely reflexive responses to the novelty of surprising
outcomes, but instead reflect deeper attempts to learn about aspects of the
world that failed to accord with expectations," said Stahl, the paper's
lead author and a doctoral student in psychological and brain sciences.
"Infants are not
only equipped with core knowledge about fundamental aspects of the world, but
from early in their lives, they harness this knowledge to empower new
learning."
The study was supported
by the National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship.
Comments
Post a Comment