HANDSHAKES MAY ENGAGE OUR SENSE OF SMELL
Why do people shake
hands? A new Weizmann Institute study suggests one of the reasons for this
ancient custom may be to check out each other's odors. Even if we are not
consciously aware of this, handshaking may provide people with a socially
acceptable way of communicating via the sense of smell
Not only do people
often sniff their own hands, but they do so for a much longer time after
shaking someone else's hand, the study has found. As reported today in the
journal eLife, the number of seconds the subjects spent sniffing
their own right hand more than doubled after an experimenter greeted them with
a handshake.
"Our findings
suggest that people are not just passively exposed to socially-significant
chemical signals, but actively seek them out," said Idan Frumin, the
research student who conducted the study under the guidance of Prof. Noam Sobel
of Weizmann's Neurobiology Department. "Rodents, dogs and other mammals
commonly sniff themselves, and they sniff one another in social interactions,
and it seems that in the course of evolution, humans have retained this
practice -- only on a subliminal level."
To examine whether
handshakes indeed transfer body odors, the researchers first had experimenters
wearing gloves shake the subjects' bare hands, then tested the glove for smell
residues. They found that a handshake alone was sufficient for the transfer of
several odors known to serve as meaningful chemical signals in mammals.
"It's well known that germs can be passed on through skin contact in
handshakes, but we've shown that potential chemical messages, known as
chemosignals, can be passed on in the same manner," Frumin says.
Next, to explore the
potential role of handshakes in communicating odors, the scientists used covert
cameras to film some 280 volunteers before and after they were greeted by an
experimenter, who either shook their hand or didn't. The researchers found that
after shaking hands with an experimenter of the same gender, subjects more than
doubled the time they later spent sniffing their own right hand (the shaking
one). In contrast, after shaking hands with an experimenter of the opposite
gender, subjects increased the sniffing of their own left hand (the non-shaking
one). "The sense of smell plays a particularly important role in
interactions within gender, not only across gender as commonly assumed,"
Frumin says.
The scientists then
performed a series of tests to make sure the hand-sniffing indeed served the
purpose of checking out odors and was not merely a stress-related response to a
strange situation. First, they measured nasal airflow during the task and found
that subjects were truly sniffing their hands and not just lifting them to
their nose. It turned out that the amount of air inhaled by the volunteers
through the nose doubled when they brought their hands to their face. Next, the
scientists found they could manipulate the hand-sniffing by artificially
introducing different smells into the experimental setting. For example, when
experimenters were tainted with a commercial unisex perfume, the hand-sniffing
increased. In contrast, when the experimenters were tainted with odors derived
from sex hormones, the sniffing decreased. These final tests confirmed the
olfactory nature of the hand-sniffing behavior.
Taking part in the
study were Ofer Perl, Yaara Endevelt-Shapira, Ami Eisen, Neetai Eshel, Iris
Heller, Maya Shemesh, Aharon Ravia, Dr. Lee Sela and Dr. Anat Arzi, all of
Prof. Sobel's lab
"Handshakes vary
in strength, duration and posture, so they convey social information of various
sorts," says Prof. Sobel. "But our findings suggest that at its
evolutionary origins, handshaking might have also served to convey odor
signals, and such signaling may still be a meaningful, albeit subliminal,
component of this custom."
Comments
Post a Comment